Open Society

Open Society

5232 bookmarks
Custom sorting
Kanye West and proving your disabilities | CNN
Kanye West and proving your disabilities | CNN
David Perry says Kanye West was practicing behavior that many of us engage in -- showing skepticism about people's disabilities.
Every day, in every context, people with disabilities get challenged to prove how disabled they are. This constant questioning isolates people with disabilities, increases stress and shame, and can lead directly to verbal or even physical abuse.
The parking lot, with its handicapped spots, can be particularly fraught for people with invisible disabilities. On my Facebook page, one of my readers remarked that, “All I see are looks of disapproval, barely veiled disgust and constant scrutiny. I’d gladly trade my ‘good’ parking space for being able to walk more than 25 yards at a time.” Who knows how many people with invisible disabilities, managing pain, enjoying the concert, were forced to their feet by the glare of Kanye West and the peer pressure of the shouting crowd? Invisible disability also gets people in trouble with the law. When a disabled person doesn’t react the way a law enforcement officer thinks he or she should, violence often follows. But visible disability is no protection. In 2008, as shown in a disturbing video, a Florida deputy didn’t believe a man who had been arrested on a traffic violation was really a quadriplegic, so she dumped him onto the floor to see if he would use his legs out of duress. These are just a few of the ways we constantly demand that people with disabilities prove themselves. We do it because, to the not-disabled, claiming disability seems to have a kind of power. Thanks to the Americans with Disabilities Act, to claim disability is to ask for reasonable accommodation – accessible buildings, more time on tests, audible formats for books, Social Security disability payments, and more. Too many people seem to regard the request to accommodate as a burden and meet such requests with suspicion. The not-disabled exercise their privilege by demanding that people prove their disabilities; then, all too often, proof just generates pity, not understanding or inclusion.
·cnn.com·
Kanye West and proving your disabilities | CNN
Hidden Disabilities; or, You Have No Idea What You Are Seeing – This is David M. Perry
Hidden Disabilities; or, You Have No Idea What You Are Seeing – This is David M. Perry
This is just one of the reasons I work so hard on how we portral/represent disability in our culture. People think they know what disabled looks like, but they have no idea. People think they know what “disabled activities” look like, what disabled people can do. So let me set you straight – disabled people, as a group, can do everything. Some can do some things. Some can do other things. Some can do some things some of the time, other things none of the time, and all things most of the time.
·davidmperry.com·
Hidden Disabilities; or, You Have No Idea What You Are Seeing – This is David M. Perry
We must improve the low standards underlying “evidence-based practice” - Kristen Bottema-Beutel, 2023
We must improve the low standards underlying “evidence-based practice” - Kristen Bottema-Beutel, 2023
Evidence-based practice is the process of identifying the best available evidence to make decisions about practices that should be deployed to support individuals in a given population (McKibbon, 1998, see Vivanti, 2022, for a review in relation to autism). Practices that meet a predefined set of evidentiary criteria are labeled “evidence-based practices” (EBPs1), to promote their adoption by service providers. A tenet of EBP is that the research used to designate EBPs should be rigorous, with the fewest risks of bias possible (Slavin, 2008).
Critics of autism EBP frameworks have argued that they: do not consider the scope of change indexed by outcome measures so that broad, developmental change and narrow, context-bound change are conflated (Sandbank et al., 2021)2; lead to an overestimation of effectiveness by tallying studies that show effects while ignoring gray literature, studies showing null effects, and studies showing iatrogenic effects (Sandbank et al., 2020; Slavin, 2008); and use taxonomies for categorizing practices that confuse practices and specific components of those practices (Ledford et al., 2021). The aim of this editorial is to point out another limitation of autism EBP frameworks, which is that research quality thresholds are much too low for making determinations about which interventions are likely to be efficacious. Low standards result in practices with questionable efficacy being labeled EBPs and promoted for use, and perpetuate the continued production of low-quality autism intervention research.
Crucially, none of these EBP frameworks considers whether intervention researchers measure or report on adverse events, which are unintended negative consequences of interventions that can cause short- or long-term harms. This is problematic because selecting interventions should involve appropriate weighting of the potential for benefit against the potential for harm. The pairing of low standards with insufficient consideration of adverse events that is common to each of these frameworks could mean that researchers routinely recommend interventions that confer little or no benefit, while also inadvertently putting autistic people at risk of harm.
Across these two reviews, we found that adverse events were rarely mentioned (they were mentioned in 7% of studies in our review on young children, and in only 2% of studies in our review on transition-age youth), but there is nevertheless evidence that they do occur (Bottema-Beutel et al., 2021a, 2022).
The conclusions from these two quality reviews starkly contrast with findings from EBP reports. For example, nearly half of the 28 practices designated as “evidence-based” in the most recent NCAEP report were behavioral (i.e. practices that rely on manipulating behavioral antecedents and consequences to shape new behavior).4 Similarly, Smith and Iadarola’s (2015) report concluded that behavioral practices either alone or in combination with developmental practices were “well established,” and the National Autism Center (2015) considered a variety of behaviorally-based interventions to be “established.” However, in Sandbank et al. (2020), we showed that there were too few randomized controlled trials of behavioral interventions to make any conclusions about their efficacy for autistic children. In our review of interventions for transition-age autistic youth (Bottema-Beutel et al., 2022), we found that although 70% of the interventions tested were behaviorally-based, quality concerns prevented us from considering any intervention practice to have sufficient evidence. Because autism EBP frameworks do not distinguish between research that adheres to some quality standards but is still designed with significant risks of bias, and research with minimized risks of bias, the reports may mislead researchers, practitioners, and commissioners of services to conclude that behavioral interventions are better supported by research evidence than other kinds of interventions, given the high number of behavioral strategies labeled as EBPs. In reality, behavioral intervention research has more risks of bias relative to research examining other types of interventions (Sandbank et al., 2020).
·journals.sagepub.com·
We must improve the low standards underlying “evidence-based practice” - Kristen Bottema-Beutel, 2023
(PDF) Autism and the ‘double empathy problem’
(PDF) Autism and the ‘double empathy problem’
PDF | On Jan 4, 2023, Damian E. M. Milton and others published Autism and the ‘double empathy problem’ | Find, read and cite all the research you need on ResearchGate
·researchgate.net·
(PDF) Autism and the ‘double empathy problem’
(99+) alwaysbewoke on Tumblr
(99+) alwaysbewoke on Tumblr
to be clear the first one is the original. i just riffed on it because it's a powerful message.
·tumblr.com·
(99+) alwaysbewoke on Tumblr
Autistic Traits and Experiences in “Love and Mercy” The Brian Wilson Story
Autistic Traits and Experiences in “Love and Mercy” The Brian Wilson Story
Utensils, noise, stress and autistic meltdowns I watched a fantastic movie over the weekend. “Love and Mercy” the Brian Wilson biopic. It tells the story of Brian Wilson, the creative s…
·peripheralmindsofautism.com·
Autistic Traits and Experiences in “Love and Mercy” The Brian Wilson Story
Physiological mechanisms of stress-induced evolution
Physiological mechanisms of stress-induced evolution
Summary: Eukaryotes can employ five mechanisms when experiencing stress to accelerate the process of adaptation. These mechanisms are outlined with emphasis on examples in animals.
·journals.biologists.com·
Physiological mechanisms of stress-induced evolution
Who is more susceptible to job stressors and resources? Sensory-processing sensitivity as a personal resource and vulnerability factor
Who is more susceptible to job stressors and resources? Sensory-processing sensitivity as a personal resource and vulnerability factor
This study aimed to investigate whether people scoring higher (compared to lower) on sensory-processing sensitivity respond differently to the work environment. Specifically, based on the literature on sensory-processing sensitivity and the Job Demands-Resources model, we predicted that the three components of sensory-processing sensitivity (i.e. ease of excitation, aesthetic sensitivity and low sensory threshold) amplify the relationship between job demands (i.e. workload and emotional demands) and emotional exhaustion as well as the relationship between job resources (i.e. task autonomy and social support) and helping behaviour. Survey data from 1019 Belgian employees were analysed using structural equation modelling analysis. The results showed that ease of excitation and low sensory threshold amplified the relationship between job demands and emotional exhaustion. Low sensory threshold also strengthened the job resources–helping behaviour relationship. This study offered first evidence on the greater susceptibility among highly sensitive persons to the work environment and demonstrated that the moderating role might differ for the three components of sensory-processing sensitivity. Additionally, it adds sensory-processing sensitivity to the Job Demands-Resources model and highlights the idea that personal factors may act both as a personal vulnerability factor and a personal resource, depending on the nature of the perceived work environment.
This study aimed to investigate whether people scoring higher (compared to lower) on sensory-processing sensitivity respond differently to the work environment. Specifically, based on the literature on sensory-processing sensitivity and the Job Demands-Resources model, we predicted that the three components of sensory-processing sensitivity (i.e. ease of excitation, aesthetic sensitivity and low sensory threshold) amplify the relationship between job demands (i.e. workload and emotional demands) and emotional exhaustion as well as the relationship between job resources (i.e. task autonomy and social support) and helping behaviour. Survey data from 1019 Belgian employees were analysed using structural equation modelling analysis. The results showed that ease of excitation and low sensory threshold amplified the relationship between job demands and emotional exhaustion. Low sensory threshold also strengthened the job resources–helping behaviour relationship. This study offered first evidence on the greater susceptibility among highly sensitive persons to the work environment and demonstrated that the moderating role might differ for the three components of sensory-processing sensitivity. Additionally, it adds sensory-processing sensitivity to the Job Demands-Resources model and highlights the idea that personal factors may act both as a personal vulnerability factor and a personal resource, depending on the nature of the perceived work environment.
Overall, the results of this study suggest that—depending on the nature of the work environment in terms of job demands–resources—SPS can be conceived as a personal vulnerability factor and a personal resource that boosts the energetic and motivational process, as outlined in the JD-R model. While EOE and LST were found to amplify the relationship between job demands (i.e. workload and emotional demands) and emotional exhaustion, only LST amplified the relationship between job resources (i.e. task autonomy and social support) and helping behaviour. This study expands our knowledge by providing evidence of the phenomenon of differential susceptibility of highly sensitive persons to their work environment, and contributes to the JD-R model by adding SPS as a new moderating trait or person-related variable to the model.
SPS has been linked to the phenomenon of differential susceptibility [10, 11]: people with high levels of SPS may react more strongly to both negative and positive stimuli. This implies that SPS may not only relate to a higher vulnerability to negative effects of adversity, but also to a disproportional susceptibility to the beneficial effects of benign situations [10]. Applying this reasoning to the context of work stress, we may expect employees with high levels of SPS to respond more strongly to both negative and positive work characteristics.
The aim of this study was to investigate the principle of differential susceptibility to cues in the work environment [10, 11] of people scoring higher rather than lower on SPS. Based on the literature on SPS [1, 7] and the JD-R model [12, 13, 24], we predicted that SPS acts as a vulnerability factor, amplifying the relationship between job demands and emotional exhaustion. At the same time, it may act as a personal resource increasing the relationship between job resources and helping behaviour. These predictions were investigated for each of the three dimensions of SPS (i.e. EOE, AES and LST) separately, in line with previous recommendations [6, 15]. The results offered first evidence for the greater susceptibility of persons with higher levels of SPS to the work context: EOE and LST amplified the positive relationship between job demands and emotional exhaustion, and LST also amplified the positive relationship between job resources and helping behaviour.
·journals.plos.org·
Who is more susceptible to job stressors and resources? Sensory-processing sensitivity as a personal resource and vulnerability factor
Does cognitive aging follow an orchid and dandelion phenomenon?
Does cognitive aging follow an orchid and dandelion phenomenon?
Cognitive reserve reflects the brain’s intrinsic adaptive capacity against the neurodegenerative effects of aging. The maintenance or enhancement of the brain’s cognitive reserve plays a crucial role in mitigating the severity of pathologies associated with aging. A new movement, social prescribing, which focuses on prescribing lifestyle activities as a treatment for patients, is growing in popularity as a solution against aging pathologies. However, few studies have demonstrated a clear impact of lifestyle activities on individual cognitive health, outside of floor and ceiling effects. Understanding who benefits from which lifestyle factors remains unclear. Here, we investigated the potential effects of lifestyle activities on individuals’ cognitive health from more than 3,530 older adults using a stratification method and advanced analysis technique. Our stratification methods allowed us to observe a new result: older adults who had relatively average cognitive scores were not impacted by lifestyle factors. By comparison, older adults with very high or very low cognitive scores were highly impacted by lifestyle factors. These findings expand the orchid and dandelion theory to the aging field, regarding the biological sensitivity of individuals to harmful and protective environmental effects. Our discoveries demonstrate the role of individual differences in the aging process and its importance for social prescribing programs.
Our stratification methods allowed us to observe a new result: older adults who had relatively average cognitive scores were not impacted by lifestyle factors. By comparison, older adults with very high or very low cognitive scores were highly impacted by lifestyle factors. These findings expand the orchid and dandelion theory to the aging field, regarding the biological sensitivity of individuals to harmful and protective environmental effects. Our discoveries demonstrate the role of individual differences in the aging process and its importance for social prescribing programs.
The possibility of this individual predisposition leads us to our second critical result–the analysis revealed dichotomous findings on individuals’ overall susceptibility to lifestyle factors. Individuals in the intermediate CC were largely resistant to the effects of lifestyle factors, be they detrimental or enriching. By comparison, individuals in the extreme CCs were especially susceptible to these same lifestyle factors. An explanation for this pattern of results may come not from gerontology but from the developmental sciences. Boyce and Ellis (2005) advanced a theory that accounts for biological sensitivities in childhood to various harmful and protective environmental effects and their impact on development into adulthood. They proposed a developmental dichotomy to describe their pediatric patients: the theory of orchids and dandelions. According to this view, orchid individuals are more environment-sensitive: they thrive under ideal conditions but are also more susceptible to deterioration in poor environmental conditions [see Boyce and Ellis (2005) and Ellis et al. (2011)]. In contrast, dandelion individuals are relatively less environment-sensitive: they do not thrive to the same degree as orchid individuals in ideal conditions but are also more resilient to deterioration in poor environmental conditions (Luthar et al., 1993; Masten, 2001). Although concepts of orchid and dandelion individuals were first developed to account for different trajectories in childhood development, the present results suggest that a similar framework may also apply at the other end of the life continuum, with more- and less-environment-sensitive older adults. The extreme cognitive categories may reflect the environment-sensitive qualities of orchid older adults. Conversely, the stability of the central cognitive score category may represent the environment-insensitive qualities of dandelion older adults.
Taken on the whole, our findings offer a new conceptualization of the aging process: the orchid and dandelion aging theory. This new framework will allow researchers to formulate new questions and hypotheses and reinterpret the literature’s critical findings. These discoveries also offer new possibilities to help and support our older populations throughout the aging process. Understanding this distribution of the aging population could help decision-makers offer older adults solutions fitting their needs instead of the current one-size-fits-all policy model. The possibility of having a significant impact on aging health policies and providing substantial evidence for new social prescribing programs is real.
·frontiersin.org·
Does cognitive aging follow an orchid and dandelion phenomenon?
Does cognitive aging follow an orchid and dandelion phenomenon? - PubMed
Does cognitive aging follow an orchid and dandelion phenomenon? - PubMed
Cognitive reserve reflects the brain's intrinsic adaptive capacity against the neurodegenerative effects of aging. The maintenance or enhancement of the brain's cognitive reserve plays a crucial role in mitigating the severity of pathologies associated with aging. A new movement, social prescribing, …
·pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov·
Does cognitive aging follow an orchid and dandelion phenomenon? - PubMed
Is Autism a Stress Adaptation of Neurodivergent Neurotypes?
Is Autism a Stress Adaptation of Neurodivergent Neurotypes?
Is Autism a Stress Adaptation of Neurodivergent Neurotypes? Author: Lori Hogenkamp, B.A. Psychology, Autistic Advocate, Director of the Center for Adaptive Stress Edited: July 24, 2018
·peripheralmindsofautism.com·
Is Autism a Stress Adaptation of Neurodivergent Neurotypes?
PSA from the actual coiner of “neurodivergent”
PSA from the actual coiner of “neurodivergent”
Yo. Many of you need to take an entire stadium of seats. Like a football arena in Texas number. I coined neurodivergent before tumblr was even a thing, like a decade or more ago, because people were...
I coined neurodivergent before tumblr was even a thing, like a decade or more ago, because people were using ‘neurodiverse’ and ‘neurodiversity’ to just mean autistic, & possibly LDs. But there’s more, like way more, ways a person can have a different yet fucking perfect dammit brain.Neurodivergent refers to neurologically divergent from typical. That’s ALL.I am multiply neurodivergent: I’m Autistic, epileptic, have PTSD, have  cluster headaches, have a chiari malformation.Neurodivergent just means a brain that diverges.Autistic people. ADHD people. People with learning disabilities. Epileptic people. People with mental illnesses. People with MS or Parkinsons or apraxia or cerebral palsy or dyspraxia or no specific diagnosis but wonky lateralization or something. That is all it means. It is not another damn tool of exclusion. It is specifically a tool of inclusion. If you don’t want to be associated with Those People, then YOU are the one who needs another word. Neurodivergent is for all of us.
·sherlocksflataffect.tumblr.com·
PSA from the actual coiner of “neurodivergent”
Neuroculture and the dangers of homogeneity
Neuroculture and the dangers of homogeneity
Today I decided to learn about monocultures. A monoculture is an environment in which a single crop is cultivated. The problem with monocultures is that a small change can destabilise the entire th…
·emergentdivergence.com·
Neuroculture and the dangers of homogeneity
Neurodiverse or Neurodivergent? It’s more than just grammar
Neurodiverse or Neurodivergent? It’s more than just grammar
The language of neurodiversity has now been with us for some time. Judy Singer coined the word “neurodiversity” more than two decades ago, and Kassiane Asasumasu (formerly Kassiane Sibley) gave us the term “neurodivergent”º. However, the language of neurodiversity is still not being used in a standard way, neither in the community, nor in practice,…
Let’s think about ethnic diversity for a moment. This is a concept that should bring people together but has instead reinforced existing prejudices through misuse.^ Ethnic diversity is a property of the whole human race, but all too often white people use both the word “ethnic”, and the word “diverse” to refer exclusively to people of colour. Consider phrases such as “the diversity hire” to describe a non-white person employed in a majority-white company. While at least a dictionary definition of “ethnic” includes reference to being in a culturally-distinct minority, there’s no such excuse for “diverse”. What we see when someone from a majority group (neurotypical people, white people in the UK) uses “diverse” to mean “unusual” is an eradication of the ethnicity or neurotype of the speaker.  They do not class themselves as a part of diversity because they do not recognise the relative unusual-ness of their own identity.  Instead, they think of themselves as “normal” and hence everyone else as “diverse”. The desire to other is strong enough to overcome the fundamental meanings of the words in question. Of course, none of this is meant to instruct individuals how they should identify personally. The language of neurodiversity might not be right for you, or your loved ones.  There is often value in using more specific language – such as “I am dyslexic” or “I have ADHD” – but in any case everyone is entitled to their own preference. If you do choose to use the language of neurodiversity, however, let’s try to get it right and avoid repeating the mistakes that have been made in the past.
·dart.ed.ac.uk·
Neurodiverse or Neurodivergent? It’s more than just grammar
Here's What Neurodiversity Is – And What It Means For Feminism
Here's What Neurodiversity Is – And What It Means For Feminism
You may have heard the word “neurodiversity” used in a medical context around disability. Here's a summary of what neurodiversity is, how it's relevant in your life – and why we can't talk about feminism without it.
·everydayfeminism.com·
Here's What Neurodiversity Is – And What It Means For Feminism
Deaf In Prison - HEARD
Deaf In Prison - HEARD
HEARD Monthly Newsletter View & subscribe to our monthly newsletters here. #DeafInPrison Fact Sheet HEARD's #DeafInPrison Fact Sheet ASL #DeafInPrison Fact Sheet Deaf Prisoner Phone Justice Campaign HEARD launched the Deaf Prisoner Phone Justice Campaign in 2012 to lobby the Federal Communications Commission (FCC) to bring an end to exorbitant prisoner telephone rates that disproportionately impact
·behearddc.org·
Deaf In Prison - HEARD
Blog Archives
Blog Archives
This summer I created and taught a course entitled Disability Justice in the Age of Mass Incarceration: Perspectives on Race, Disability, Law & Accountability.  Here, I am providing my course...
·talilalewis.com·
Blog Archives
What School Ought to Teach (WSOT) list / Czego Powinna Uczyć Szkoła (CPUS) - Holistic Think Thank
What School Ought to Teach (WSOT) list / Czego Powinna Uczyć Szkoła (CPUS) - Holistic Think Thank
1. How to confront themselves with challenges  Including especially: problem formulation, that is, being able to formulate a problem at many levels of abstraction, presenting it in a concise form that facilitates its further analysis and with a view to solving it later on by employing different methods and tools; problem decomposition, that is, being...
·holisticthinktank.com·
What School Ought to Teach (WSOT) list / Czego Powinna Uczyć Szkoła (CPUS) - Holistic Think Thank
Five Not-So-Obvious Propositions About Play (##)
Five Not-So-Obvious Propositions About Play (##)
November 15, 2011 Five Not-So-Obvious Propositions About Play By Alfie Kohn Children should have plenty of opportunities to play. Even young children have too few such opportunities these days, particularly in school settings. These two
·alfiekohn.org·
Five Not-So-Obvious Propositions About Play (##)
(PDF) Understanding Contemporary Forms of Exploitation: Attributions of Passion Serve to Legitimize the Poor Treatment of Workers
(PDF) Understanding Contemporary Forms of Exploitation: Attributions of Passion Serve to Legitimize the Poor Treatment of Workers
PDF | The pursuit of passion in one's work is touted in contemporary discourse. Although passion may indeed be beneficial in many ways, we suggest that... | Find, read and cite all the research you need on ResearchGate
·researchgate.net·
(PDF) Understanding Contemporary Forms of Exploitation: Attributions of Passion Serve to Legitimize the Poor Treatment of Workers